Violence and video games

Source: Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia.

Since their inception in the 1970s, video games have often been criticized by some for violent content. Politicians, parents, and other activists have claimed that violence in video games can be tied to violent behavior, particularly in children, and have sought ways to regulate the sale of video games. Studies have shown no connection between video games and violent behavior. The American Psychological Association states that while there is a well-established link between violent video games and aggressive behaviors, attributing acts of violence to violent video gaming "is not scientifically sound."[1][2]

Background

Since the late 1990s, acts of violence have been highly publicized about beliefs that the suspect in the crime may have had a history of playing violent video games. The 1999 Columbine High School massacre created a moral panic around video games, spurring research to see if violent video games lead to aggressive behaviors in real life.[3] Some research finds that violent video game use is correlated with, and may cause, increases in aggression and decreases in prosocial behavior.[4][5] Other research argues that there are no such effects of violent video games.[6] This link between violent video games and antisocial behavior was denied by the president of the Interactive Digital Software Association in 2005 in a PBS interview. In the interview, he stated that the problem is "vastly overblown and overstated" by people who "don't understand, frankly, this industry".[7][failed verification] Others have theorized that there are positive effects of playing video games, including prosocial behavior in some contexts,[8][9] and argue that the video game industry has been used as a scapegoat for more generalized problems affecting some communities.[10][11][12]

A primary concern about a potential relationship between violence and video games is that a significant portion of gamers is young, and as such, particularly impressionable.[7] A large-scale study conducted in 2022 suggested that at least a quarter of all gamers is between 10 and 20 years of age.[13] Additionally, a survey of 1,102 children between 12 and 17 years of age found that 97% are video game players who have played on the last day, of which 14% of girls and 50% of boys favored games with an "M" (mature) or "AO" (adult-only) rating[14]—and 25% of parents do not check the censor's rating on a video game before allowing their child to purchase it.

History

Before video games

Elements of the type of moral panic that came with video games after they gained popularity had previously been seen with comic books. Through the 1950s, comics were in their Golden Age, having become a widely popular form of media. As the media expanded, some artists and publishers took more risks with violent and otherwise questionable content. Fredric Wertham, a psychiatrist, wrote Seduction of the Innocent in 1954, which outlined his studies asserting that violent comics were a negative form of literature and led to juvenile delinquency. Even though some of Wertham's claims were later found to be based on bad studies, the book created a moral panic that put pressure on the comic book industry to regulate their works. Later in 1954, the comic industry issued the Comics Code Authority (CCA) which put strict regulations on content that could appear in comic books sold at most stores, eliminating most violence and other mature content via self-censoring. The mainstream comic industries waned as comics had lost their edge, while an underground market for the more adult comics formed. The comic industry did not recover from Comics Code Authority regulations until the 1970s, when adherence to the Authority was weakened. By the 2000s, the Authority was generally no longer considered.[15] Modern trends of targeting violence in video games have been compared to these events in the comic industry, and video game industry leaders have specifically avoided the use of self-censorship that could impact the performance of the industry.[16][17]

Pinball machines had also created a moral panic in post-World War II America, as the teenage rebels of the 1950s and 1960s would frequently hang around establishments with pinball machines, which created fear across the generation gap of older Americans unsure of the intents of this younger crowd. To some, it appeared to be a form of gambling (which led to machines being labeled "For Amusement Only"), while more religious people feared pinball was a "tool of the devil". Because of this, many cities and towns banned pinball machines or implemented strict licensing requirements which were slowly lifted in the late 1960s and early 1970s. Notably, New York City's ban on pinball machines lasted until 1976,[18] while Chicago's was lifted in 1977.[19] The appearance of video games in the early 1970s overlapped with the lifting of bans on pinball machines, and when youth were drawn to arcade games, the same concerns that were initially leveled at pinball machines as gambling machines and immoral playthings were also made about video games.[20]

1970s–1980s

After Pong exploded onto the arcade game market, arcade game manufacturers were aware of the attention that video games were getting and tried to position games as entertainment aimed at adults, selling units preferably to bars and lounges.[20] This gave them more leeway with content, but still which drew criticism from some. Two arcade games had already drawn attention for amoral content prior to 1976. Atari's Gotcha in 1973, a maze game, initially shipped with two joystick units that were covered in pink domes as to represent women's breasts, but which were removed in later makes. The 1975 Shark Jaws, also by Atari, was an unlicensed adaption of the film Jaws and attempted to play on the film's violent context, though here, the player was hunted by the shark.[20] As arcade games spread into more locations, the ease for children to access the games also elevated concerns about their potential impacts.[20]

The 1976 arcade game Death Race is considered the first game to be targeted for its violent content. The game, like Shark Jaws, was an unlicensed adaption of the 1975 film Death Race 2000, a violent film centered on driving. Within the game, the player was challenged to drive a car and run over simulated gremlins scoring points for doing so. Besides the game's simulated content, the game cabinet was also adorned with imagery of death.[20] The game caught the attention of an Associated Press writer, Wendy Walker, who had contacted the game's manufacturer, Exidy, with her concerns that the game was excessively violent.[21] Walker's concerns spread through other media organizations, including the National Safety Council, who accused the game of glorifying the act of running people over when at the time they were trying to educate drivers about safe driving practices. While some arcades subsequently returned the Death Race machines due to this panic, sales of the game continued to grow due to the media coverage.[22] It was recognized that many other competing arcade games at the time, like Cops 'n' Robbers, Tank 8, and Jet Fighter, all games equally about violent actions, saw little complaint. Nolan Bushnell of Atari said that "We Atari had an internal rule that we wouldn't allow violence against people. You could blow up a tank or you could blow up a flying saucer, but you couldn't blow up people. We felt that that was not good form, and we adhered to that all during my tenure."[20]

United States Surgeon General C. Everett Koop was one of the first to raise concerns about the potential connection of video games to youth behavior. In 1982, Koop stated as a personal observation that "more and more people are beginning to understand" the connection between video games and mental and physical health effects on youth, though that at that time, there was not sufficient evidence to make any conclusion.[23][24]

1990s

Mortal Kombat and congressional hearings (1993–1994)

The fighting game Mortal Kombat was released into arcades in 1992. It was one of the first games to depict a large amount of blood and gore, particularly during special moves known as "Fatalities" used to finish off the losing character. Numerous arcade games that used high amounts of violent content followed in Mortal Kombat's wake. However, as these games were originally exclusive to arcade machines, it was generally possible to segregate them away from games aimed for younger players.[25][26] Eventually, there was significant interest from home console manufacturers in licensing Mortal Kombat from Midway Games, particularly from Sega for its Sega Genesis platform and Nintendo for the Super Nintendo Entertainment System. At the time, Sega and Nintendo were in the midst of a console war to try to gain dominance in the United States market.[27] Sega's licensed version of Mortal Kombat retained all the gore from the arcade version (though required a use of a cheat code to activate it), while Nintendo had a version developed that removed most of the gore, recoloring the blood as grey "sweat" and otherwise toning down the game. Sega's version drastically outsold Nintendo's version and intensified the competition between the two companies.[26]

The popularity of Mortal Kombat, along with the full-motion video game Night Trap and the light gun shooting game Lethal Enforcers, gained attention from U.S. Senators Joe Lieberman and Herb Kohl. This resulted in two congressional hearings in 1993 and 1994 to discuss the issues of violence and video games with concerned advocacy groups, academics, and the video game industry.[26] Sega, Nintendo, and others were criticized for lacking a standardized content rating system, and Lieberman threatened to have Congress pass legislation requiring a system that would have government oversight if the industry did not take its own steps.[28] By the time of the second hearing, Sega, Nintendo, and other console manufacturers had outlined their agreed-upon approach for a voluntary rating system through the Entertainment Software Ratings Board (ESRB), which was in place by the end of 1994.[29][30] This also led to the establishment of the Interactive Digital Software Association, later known as the Entertainment Software Association (ESA), a trade group for the video game industry that managed the ESRB and further supported trade-wide aspects such as government affairs.[30][31]

Jack Thompson lawsuits (1997)

Disbarred American attorney Jack Thompson has criticized a number of video games for perceived obscenity and campaigned against their producers and distributors. He argues that violent video games have repeatedly been used by teenagers as "murder simulators" to rehearse violent plans. He has pointed to alleged connections between such games and a number of school massacres.

Columbine High School massacre (1999)

The Columbine High School massacre on April 20, 1999, reignited the debate about violence in video games. Among other factors, the perpetrators, Eric Harris and Dylan Klebold, were found to be avid players of violent games like Doom. The public perceived a connection between video games and the shooting, leading to a Congressional hearing and President Bill Clinton ordering an investigation into school shootings and how video games were being marketed to youth.[32] The report, released in 2004 by the United States Secret Service and the United States Department of Education, found that 12% of perpetrators in school shootings had shown interest in video games.[32][33]

In the aftermath of the Columbine shooting, previous school shootings were re-evaluated by media and connections were drawn between Columbine and the 1998 Westside Middle School shooting. Although video games had not been identified as a factor at the time of the Westside shooting, media discussions of Columbine pointed to Westside as a similar case in that the two student perpetrators had often played GoldenEye 007 together and had enjoyed playing first-person shooter games prior to the shooting.[34][35]

2000s

Grand Theft Auto III and further lawsuits

In 2001, Rockstar Games released the PlayStation 2 game Grand Theft Auto III. The game gave the player control of a protagonist named Claude in a contemporary urban setting taking on missions within the city's criminal underworld. The game was one of the first open world games and allowed the player to have nearly free control of how they completed missions, which included gunplay, melee combat, and reckless driving. The game was widely successful, selling over two million units within six months.[36] Its popularity led several groups to criticize the violence in the game, among other factors.[37][38] Rockstar subsequently released two follow-up games, Grand Theft Auto: Vice City in 2002 and Grand Theft Auto: San Andreas in 2004, the latter becoming controversial for the sexually explicit Hot Coffee mod. After this incident, the government decided to take action. In 2005, California banned the sale of violent video games to minors.[39]

In the years that followed, a number of fatal murders and other crimes committed by young adults and youth were found to have ties to Grand Theft Auto III and later games that followed in its footsteps. Jack Thompson became involved in trying to sue Rockstar, its publisher Take-Two Interactive, and Sony on behalf of the victims for large amounts of damages, asserting that the violence in these games led directly to the crimes. Thus these companies were responsible for said crimes. These cases ultimately did not lead to any action against Rockstar, as they were either voluntarily withdrawn or dismissed before judgment. Thompson agreed to no longer seek legal action against Take-Two's games and ultimately became an activist to highlight the issues of violence in video games.[40] The events of this period were made into a BBC docudrama, The Gamechangers, which was first broadcast in September 2015.

Winnenden school shooting (2009)

The shooter in the Winnenden school shooting on March 11, 2009, in Winnenden, Germany, was found to have had interest in video games like Counter-Strike and Far Cry 2.[41][42] In the weeks that followed, politicians and concerned citizens tried to pressure the government into passing legislation to ban the sale of violent video games in the country, though this never came to pass.[43][44][45]

Call of Duty: Modern Warfare 2's "No Russian" (2009)

The 2009 first-person shooter Call of Duty: Modern Warfare 2 included a controversial mission in its story mode called "No Russian". In the mission, the player takes on the role of a CIA agent who has embedded himself among a Russian ultranationalist terrorist group; the leader of the group warns them to speak "no Russian" to give away their origins. The mission allows the player to participate in a terrorist attack at a Moscow airport, during which they may fire indiscriminately on civilians and security alike. Participation in the mission is not mandatory: a disclaimer before the mission begins warns the player about the violent content and gives the option to skip the level. If the player chooses to play the level, they are not required to participate in the shooting in order to complete the level. The level ends when the terrorist group's leader kills the player-character in order to frame the attack as the work of the United States, leading to a world war.

The existence of the level leaked before the game's release, forcing publisher Activision and developer Infinity Ward to respond to journalists and activists that were critical of the concept of the mission. Activision defended the level's inclusion in the finished game, emphasizing that the mission was not representative of the rest of the game and that initial assessments had taken the level out of context.[46][47] Even with the full game's release, "No Russian" was still criticized, with some stating that video games had yet to mature.[48] The mission is considered a watershed moment for the video game industry, in how certain depictions of violence can be seen as acceptable while others, like "No Russian", are considered unacceptable.[49][50]

2010s

Brown v. Entertainment Merchants Association (2011)

To address violent video games, several U.S. states passed laws that restricted the sale of mature video games, particularly those with violent or sexual content, to children. Video game industry groups fought these laws in courts and won.[51][52] The most significant case came out of a challenge to a California law passed in 2005 that banned the sale of mature games to minors as well as requiring an enhanced content rating system beyond the ESRB's. Industry groups fought this and won, but the case ultimately made it to the Supreme Court of the United States. In Brown v. Entertainment Merchants Association, the Supreme Court ruled that video games were a protected form of speech, qualifying for First Amendment protections, and laws like California's that block sales on a basis outside of the Miller test were unconstitutional.[53] Justice Antonin Scalia, who wrote the majority opinion, considered that violence in many video games was no different from that presented in other children's media, such as Grimm's Fairy Tales.[54]

Sandy Hook Elementary School shooting (2012)

The Sandy Hook Elementary School shooting occurred on December 14, 2012. The perpetrator, Adam Lanza, was found to have a "trove" of video games, as described by investigating officials, including several games considered to be violent.[55] This discovery started a fresh round of calls against violent video games in political and media circles,[56][57][58] including a meeting on the topic between U.S. Vice President Joe Biden and representatives from the video game industry.[59] The National Rifle Association of America blamed the video game industry for the shooting, identifying games that focused on shooting people in schools.[60][61]

Munich Olympia Mall shooting (2016)

The 2016 Munich shooting occurred on July 22, 2016, in the vicinity of the Olympia Shopping Mall in the Moosach District of Munich, Bavaria, Germany. The perpetrator, David Sonboly, killed 9 people before killing himself when surrounded by police. As a result, the German Minister of the Interior, Thomas de Maizière, claimed that the "intolerable extent of video games on the internet" has a harmful effect on the development of young people.[62] His statements were criticized by media specialist Maic Mausch, who said with regards to Maiziere's statement that "No sensible scientist can say that with such certainty. And if no scientist can do it, no minister can do that."[63]

Parkland school shooting (2018)

The Stoneman Douglas High School shooting occurred on February 14, 2018, in Parkland, Florida. In the aftermath, Kentucky Governor Matt Bevin declared that the country should re-evaluate "the things being put in the hands of our young people",[64] specifically "quote-unquote video games" that "have desensitized people to the value of human life".[65] A month later, President Donald Trump called for several industry representatives and advocates to meet in Washington, D.C. to discuss the impact of violent video games with him and his advisors. Industry leaders included Michael Gallagher, ESA president; Patricia Vance, ESRB president; Robert Altman, CEO of ZeniMax Media; and Strauss Zelnick, CEO of Take-Two, while advocates included Brent Bozell, of the Media Research Center and Melissa Henson of the Parents Television Council. While the video game industry asserted the lack of connection between violent video games and violent acts, their critics asserted that the industry should take steps to limit youth access and marketing to violent video games in ways similar to the approaches taken for alcohol and tobacco use.[66]

Suzano school shooting (2019)

The Suzano school shooting occurred on March 13, 2019, at the Professor Raul Brasil State School in the Brazilian municipality of Suzano, São Paulo. The perpetrators, Guilherme Taucci Monteiro and Luiz Henrique de Castro, managed to kill five school students and two school employees before Monteiro killed Castro and then committed suicide. As a result, Brazilian Vice President Hamilton Mourão claimed that young people are addicted to violent video games, while also claiming that the work routine of Brazilian parents made it harder for young people to be raised properly.[67] As a result, the hashtag #SomosGamersNãoAssassinos (“#WeAreGamersNotMurderers”) gained popularity in Brazil.[68]

August 2019 shootings

Two mass shootings occurring within a day of each other, one in El Paso, Texas and another in Dayton, Ohio, in August 2019 provoked political claims that video games were partially to blame for the incidents. U.S. President Donald Trump stated days after the shootings, "We must stop the glorification of violence in our society. This includes the gruesome and grisly video games that are now commonplace".[69] House Minority Leader Kevin McCarthy also blamed video games for these events, stating, "I've always felt that it's a problem for future generations and others. We've watched from studies, shown before, what it does to individuals, and you look at these photos of how it took place, you can see the actions within video games and others."[32] News organizations and the video game industry reiterated the findings of the past, that there was no link between video games and violent behavior, and criticized politicians for putting video games to task when the issues lied within proper gun control.[69][32]

Halle synagogue shooting (2019)

The Halle synagogue shooting occurred on October 9, 2019, in Halle, Saxony-Anhalt, Germany, continuing in nearby Landsberg. The suspect, identified by the media as Stephan Baillet, was influenced by far-right ideology and managed to live-stream his attack on Facebook and Twitch.[70] In the process of the attack, he managed to kill two people before being subdued by police. Given the live-streamed nature of the attack, German Minister of the Interior Horst Seehofer claimed that "many of the perpetrators or the potential perpetrators come from the gaming scene" with regards to incidents like the shooting in Halle.[71] His comments received widespread criticism from German gamers and politicians, such as SPD general secretary Lars Klingbeil, who stated that "The problem is right-wing extremism, not gamers or anything else."[72]

2020s

School shooting in Torreon, Mexico (2020)

Hours after a school shooting in Torreón, Coahuila, Mexico, in January 2020, the governor of that state, Miguel Ángel Riquelme Solís, stated that the 11-year-old shooter was wearing a T-shirt with the logo of Natural Selection, a mod for the game Half-Life, and claimed he could have been influenced by the game.[73] The governor's comment sparked a debate about the link between violence and video games. Erik Salazar Flores of the College of Psychology of the National Autonomous University of Mexico (UNAM) stated that blaming video games for violence is an "easy way out" for authorities who wish to ignore the complexity of the problem.[74] Dalila Valenzuela, a sociologist from Autonomous University of Baja California said that while video games can influence children's behavior, their parents are most directly responsible.[75]

Nahel Merzouk protests (2023)

In June 2023, Nahel Merzouk, a 17 year old of North African descent, was killed by police in the Parisian suburb of Nanterre. As protests and riots gripped France in the aftermath, President Emmanuel Macron criticized what he called the "intoxicating effect" of video games and social media for the civil unrest gripping the country.[76]

Studies

The policy statement of the American Psychological Association (APA) related to video games states "Scant evidence has emerged that makes any causal or correlational connection between playing violent video games and actually committing violent activities."[69] The APA has acknowledged that violent video games strongly correlates with aggressive behavior, as well as anti-social behavior, but distinguishes between aggression and violence. A 2015 APA review of current studies in this area described the link between violent video games and aggressive behavior as "both as an increase in negative outcomes such as aggressive behavior, cognition, and affect and as a decrease in positive outcomes such as prosocial behavior, empathy, and sensitivity to aggression."[77] However, the APA recognized the studies tended to be disproportionate to normal demographics.[77]

Further, the APA issued a policy statement in 2017 aimed at politicians and media to urge them to avoid linking violent video games with violent crimes, reiterating the subject of their findings over the years.[78] In a follow-up statement in 2020, the APA reaffirmed that there remains insufficient evidence to link video games to violent behavior. They had found that there was "small, reliable association between violent video game use and aggressive outcomes, such as yelling and pushing," but could not extend that to more violent activities.[79]

Christopher Ferguson, a professor at Stetson University and a researcher on the connection between violent video games and violent behavior, has stated that "[t]here's not evidence of a correlation, let alone a causation" between videogames and violence.[80] Ferguson's more recent studies have shown that there is no predictive behavior that can be inferred from the playing of violent video games.[81][82]

A longitudinal study published in Molecular Psychiatry in 2021 found no significant changes in semantic accessibility to aggressive words, frustration tolerance, empathy, or prosocial behavior between those who played a violent video game and those who played a non-violent video game daily for two months.[83] Similarly, a 2023 study published in eLife found that playing violent video games, specifically "Grand Theft Auto V," does not decrease empathy in players. The research used fMRI and behavioral tests to measure empathy and found no significant evidence of reduced empathetic responses or emotional reactions to violence in players.[84]

Negative effects of violent video games

Theories of negative effects of video games tend to focus on players' modeling of behaviors observed in the game. These effects may be exacerbated due to the interactive nature of these games. The most well-known theory of such effects is the cognitive neo-association theory (also referred to as Script Theory), which proposes that playing violent video games may create cognitive scripts of aggression which will be activated in incidents in which individuals think others are acting with hostility.[85] Playing violent video games, thus, becomes an opportunity to rehearse acts of aggression, which then become more common in real life. The General Aggression Model, a meta-theoretical framework to understand how various theories work together to explain aggressive behaviors, incorporates various theories suggesting that the simulated violence of video games may influence players' thoughts, feelings and physical arousal, affecting individuals' interpretation of others' behavior and increasing their own aggressive behavior.[86] Some scholars have criticized the general aggression model, arguing that the model wrongly assumes that aggression is primarily learned and that the brain does not distinguish reality from fiction.[87] Some recent studies have explicitly claimed to find evidence against the effect of short-term exposure to violent video games on aggression.[88][89][90]

Parents can protect their children from violence used in video games by limiting game usage and privileges. Some biological theories of aggression have specifically excluded video game and other media effects because the evidence for such effects is considered weak and the impact too distant. For example, the catalyst model of aggression comes from a diathesis-stress perspective, implying that aggression is due to a combination of genetic risk and environmental strain. The catalyst model suggests that stress, coupled with antisocial personality are salient factors leading to aggression. It does allow that proximal influences such as family or peers may alter aggressiveness but not media and games.[91][92]

Research methods

Research has focused on two elements of the effects of video games on players: the player's health measures and educational achievements as a function of game play amounts; the players' behavior or perceptions as a function of the game's violence levels;[93] the context of the game play in terms of group dynamics; the game's structure which affects players' visual attention or three dimensional constructional skills; and the mechanics of the game which affects hand–eye coordination.[94] Two other research methods that have been used are experimental (in a laboratory), where the different environmental factors can be controlled, and non-experimental, where those who participate in studies simply log their video gaming hours.[4]

Scientific debate

A common theory is that playing violent video games increases aggression in young people. Various studies claim to support this hypothesis.[4][95][96] Other studies find no link.[97][98] Debate among scholars on both sides remains contentious, and there is argument about whether consensus exists regarding the effects of violent video games on aggression.[99][100]

Primary studies

In 1998, Steven Kirsh reported in the journal Childhood that the use of video games may lead to acquisition of a hostile attribution bias. Fifty-five subjects were randomized to play either violent or non-violent video games. Subjects were later asked to read stories in which the characters' behaviour was ambiguous. Participants randomized to play violent video games were more likely to provide negative interpretations of the stories.[101] Another study done by Anderson and Dill in 2000 found a correlation in undergraduate students between playing violent video games and violent crime, with the correlation stronger in aggressive male players,[102] although other scholars have suggested that results from this study were not consistent, and that the methodology was flawed.[103]

In 2001, David Satcher, the Surgeon General of the United States, said "We clearly associate media violence to aggressive behavior. But the impact was very small compared to other things. Some may not be happy with that, but that's where the science is."[104]

A 2002 US Secret Service study of 41 individuals who had been involved in school shootings found that twelve percent were attracted to violent video games, twenty-four percent read violent books and twenty-seven percent were attracted to violent films.[105] Some scholars have indicated that these numbers are unusually low compared to violent media consumption among non-criminal youth.[106]

In 2003, a study was conducted at Iowa State University assessing pre-existing attitudes and violence in children.[107] The study concerned children between ages 5 and 12 that were assessed for the typical amount of time they played video games per week and pre-existing empathy and attitudes towards violence. The children played a violent or non-violent video game for approximately 15 minutes. Afterwards, their pulse rates were recorded, and the children were asked how frustrating the games were on a 1-10 scale. Last, the children are given drawings (vignettes) of everyday situations, some more likely to have aggressive actions following the depiction, while others an empathetic action. Results show that there were no significant effects of video game playing in the short term, with violent video games and non-violent video games having no significant differences, indicating that children do not have decreased empathy from playing violent video games. Conversely, children who play more violent video games over a long period of time were associated with lower pre-existing empathy, and also lower scores on the empathy inducing vignettes, indicating long-term effects. It is possible that video games had not primed children for the particular aggression scenarios. This data could indicate desensitization in children can occur after long-term exposure, but not all children were affected in the same way, so the researchers deduced that some children may be at a higher risk of these negative effects. It is possible that fifteen minutes is not quite long enough to produce short-term cognitive effects.

In 2003, Jeanne B. Funk and her colleagues at the Department of Psychology at the University of Toledo examined the relationship between exposure to violence through media and real-life, and desensitization (reflected by loss of empathy and changes in attitudes toward violence) in fourth and fifth grade pupils. Funk found that exposure to video game violence was associated with lowered empathy and stronger proviolence attitudes.[108]

Another study from 2003, by John Colwell at the University of Westminster, found that violent video game playing was associated with reduced aggression among Japanese youth.[109]

The American Psychological Association (APA) released an official statement in 2005, which said that exposure to violent media increases feelings of hostility, thoughts about aggression, suspicions about the motives of others, and demonstrates violence as a method to deal with potential conflict situations, that comprehensive analysis of violent interactive video game research suggests such exposure increases aggressive behavior, thoughts, angry feelings, physiological arousal, and decreases helpful behavior, and that studies suggest that sexualized violence in the media has been linked to increases in violence towards women, rape myth acceptance and anti-women attitudes. It also states that the APA advocates reduction of all violence in videogames and interactive media marketed to children and youth, that research should be made regarding the role of social learning, sexism, negative depiction of minorities, and gender on the effects of violence in video games and interactive media on children, adolescents, and young adults, and that it engages those responsible for developing violent video games and interactive media in addressing the issue that playing violent video games may increase aggressive thoughts and aggressive behaviors in children, youth, and young adults, and that these effects may be greater than the well documented effects of exposure to violent television and movies. They also recommend to the entertainment industry that the depiction of the consequences of violent behavior be associated with negative social consequences and that they support a rating system which accurately reflects the content of video games and interactive media. The statement was updated in 2015 (see below.)

Some scholars suggested that the APA's policy statement ignored discrepant research and misrepresented the scientific literature.[110][111] In 2013 a group of over 230 media scholars wrote an open letter to the APA asking them to revisit and greatly amend their policy statement on video game violence, due to considering the evidence to be mixed. Signatories to the 2013 letter included psychologists Jeffrey Arnett, Randy Borum, David Buss, David Canter, Lorenza Colzato, M. Brent Donnellan, Dorothy Espelage, Frank Farley, Christopher Ferguson, Peter Gray, Mark D. Griffiths, Jessica Hammer, Mizuko Ito, James C. Kaufman, Dana Klisanin, Catherine McBride-Chang, Jean Mercer, Hal Pashler, Steven Pinker, Richard M. Ryan, Todd K. Shackelford, Daniel Simons, Ian Spence, and Dean Simonton, criminologists Kevin Beaver, James Alan Fox, Roger J.R. Levesque, and Mike A. Males, game design researchers Bob De Schutter and Kurt Squire, communications scholar Thorsten Quandt, and science writer Richard Rhodes.[112][113]

In 2005, a study by Bruce D. Bartholow and colleagues at the University of Missouri, University of Michigan, Vrije Universiteit, and University of North Carolina using event related potential linked video game violence exposure to brain processes hypothetically reflecting desensitization. The authors suggested that chronic exposure to violent video games have lasting harmful effects on brain function and behavior.[114]

In 2005, a study at Iowa State University, the University of Michigan, and Vrije Universiteit by Nicholas L. Carnagey and colleagues found that participants who had previously played a violent video game had lower heart rate and galvanic skin response while viewing filmed real violence, demonstrating a physiological desensitization to violence.[115]

In 2007, a study at the Swinburne University of Technology found that children had variable reactions to violent games, with some kids becoming more aggressive, some becoming less aggressive, but the majority showing no changes in behavior.[116]

In 2008, a longitudinal study conducted in Japan assessed possible long-term effects of video game playing in children.[117] The final analysis consisted of 591 fifth graders aged 10–11 across eight public elementary schools, and was conducted over the course of a year. Initially, children were asked to complete a survey which assessed presence or absence of violence in the children's favorite video games, as well as video game context variables that may affect the results and the aggression levels of the children. Children were assessed again for these variables a year later. Results reveal that there is a significant difference in gender, with boys showing significantly more aggressive behavior and anger than girls, which was attributed by the authors to boys elevated interest in violent video games. However the interaction between time spent gaming and preference for violent games was associated with reduced aggression in boys but not girls. The researchers also found that eight context variables they assessed increased aggression, including unjustified violence, availability of weapons, and rewards. Three context variables, role-playing, extent of violence, and humor, were associated with decreased aggression. It is unknown if the observed changes from the two surveys are actually contextual effects. The researchers found that the context and quality of the violence in video games affects children more than simply presence and amount of violence, and these effects are different from child to child.

In 2008 the Pew Internet and American Life Project statistically examined the impact of video gaming on youths' social and communal behaviors. Teens who had communal gaming experiences reported much higher levels of civic and political engagement than teens who had not had these kinds of experiences. Youth who took part in social interaction related to the game, such as commenting on websites or contributing to discussion boards, were more engaged communally and politically. Among teens who play games, 63% reported seeing or hearing "people being mean and overly aggressive while playing," 49% reported seeing or hearing "people being hateful, racist or sexist while playing", and 78% reported witnessing "people being generous or helpful while playing".[118][119]

In 2009, a report of three studies conducted among students of different age groups in Singapore, Japan, and the United States, found that prosocial mostly nonviolent games increased helpful prosocial behaviour among the participants.[120]

In 2010, Patrick and Charlotte Markey suggested that violent video games only caused aggressive feelings in individuals who had a preexisting disposition, such as high neuroticism, low agreeableness, or low conscientiousness.[121]

In 2010, after a review of the effects of violent video games, the Attorney General's Office of Australia reported that even though the Anderson meta-analysis of 2010 was the pinnacle of the scientific debate at that time, significant harm from violent video games had not been persuasively proven or disproven, except that there was some consensus that they might be harmful to people with aggressive or psychotic personality traits.[122]

The attorney general considered a number of issues including:

  • Social and political controversy about the topic.
  • Lack of consensus about definitions and measures of aggression and violent video games (for example, whether a cartoon game has the same impact as a realistic one).
  • Levels of aggression may or may not be an accurate marker for the likelihood of violent behaviour.
  • The playing of violent video games may not be an independent variable in determining violent acts (for example, violent behaviour after playing violent video games may be age dependant, or players of violent video games may watch other violent media).
  • Studies may not have been long or large enough to provide clear conclusions.[122]

In 2010, researchers Paul Adachi and Teena Willoughby at Brock University critiqued experimental video game studies on both sides of the debate, noting that experimental studies often confounded violent content with other variables such as competitiveness.[123] In a follow up study, the authors found that competitiveness but not violent content was associated with aggression.[124]

In 2011, a thirty-year study of 14,000 college students, published by the University of Michigan which measured overall empathy levels in students, found that these had dropped by 40% since the 1980s. The biggest drop came after the year 2000, which the authors speculated was due to multiple factors, including increased societal emphasis on selfishness, changes in parenting practices, increased isolation due to time spent with information technology, and greater immersion in all forms of violent and/or narcissistic media including, but not limited to, news, television and video games. The authors did not provide data on media effects, but referenced various research of the topics.[125]

In 2011, in a longitudinal study of youth in Germany, von Salisch found that aggressive children tend to select more violent video games. This study found no evidence that violent games caused aggression in minors. The author speculated that other studies may have been affected by "single responder bias" due to self-reporting of aggression rather than reporting by parents or teachers.[126]

In 2012 a Swedish study examined the cooperative behavior of players in The Lord of the Rings Online. The authors argued that attempts to link collaborative or aggressive behavior within the game to real life behavior would rely on unwarranted assumptions regarding equivalencies of forms of cooperation and the material conditions of the environment in-game and out-of-game.[127]

One study from Morgan Tear and Mark Nielsen in 2013 concluded that violent video games did not reduce or increase prosocial behavior, failing to replicated previous studies in this area.[98]

In 2013, Isabela Granic and colleagues at Radboud University Nijmegen, the Netherlands, argued that even violent video games may promote learning, health, and social skills, but that not enough games had been developed to treat mental health problems. Granic et al. noted that both camps have valid points, and a more balanced perspective and complex picture is necessary.[128]

In 2014, Ferguson and Olson found no correlation between video game violence and bullying or delinquency in children with preexisting attention deficit disorder or depressive symptoms.[129]

In 2014, Villanova professor Patrick M. Markey conducted a study with 118 teenagers suggesting that video games have no influence on increased aggression of users; however, he did find that when used for the right amount of time (roughly 1 hour) video games can make children nicer and more socially interactive. This information was provided by the teens teachers at their local schools.[130][unreliable source?]

A 2014 study by Andrew Przybylski at Oxford University examined the impact of violent content and frustration on hostility among video game players. In a series of experiments, Przybylski and colleagues demonstrated that frustration, but not violent content, increased player hostility. The authors also demonstrated that some previous "classic" violent video game experiments were difficult to replicate.[131]

One longitudinal study from 2014 suggested that violent video games were associated with very small increases in risk taking behavior over time.[132]

In 2015, the American Psychological Association released a review that found that violent video games caused aggressive behavior, with Mark Appelbaum, the chair of the task force that conducted the review, saying that "the link between violence in video games and increased aggression in players is one of the most studied and best established in the field." However, Appelbaum also characterized the size of the correlation as "not very big". The same review found insufficient evidence of a link between such video games and crime or delinquency. Critics, including Peter Gray and Christopher Ferguson, expressed concerns about methodological limitations of the review. Ferguson stated that "I think (the task force members) were selected because their opinions were pretty clear going in." At least four of the seven task force members had previously expressed opinions on the topic; critics argued this alone constitutes a conflict of interest, while a task force member defended that "If it were common practice to exclude all scientists after they render one conclusion, the field would be void of qualified experts".[133][134]

A 2015 study examined the impact of violent video games on young adults players with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). The study found no evidence for an impact of playing such games on aggression among ASD players. These results appeared to contradict concerns following the 2012 Sandy Hook shooting, that individuals with ASD or other mental conditions might be particularly susceptible to violent video game effects.[135]

One study from 2016 suggested that "sexist" games (using games from the Grand Theft Auto series as exemplars) may reduce empathy toward women. Although no direct game effect was found, the authors argued that an interaction between game condition, masculine role norms, gender and avatar identification produced enough evidence to claim causal effects. Comments by other scholars on this study reflect some concerns over the methodology including a possible failure of the randomization to game conditions (see comments tab).[136]

In 2016, a preregistered study of violent video game effects concluded that violent video games did not influence aggression in players.[90] The preregistered nature of the study removed the potential for the scholars to "nudge" the results of the study in favor of the hypothesis and suggests that preregistration of future studies may help clarify results in the field.

Meta-analyses

Because the results of individual studies have often reached different conclusions, debate has often shifted to the use of meta-analysis. This method attempts to average across individual studies, determine whether there is some effect on average, and test possible explanations for differences between study results.

A number of meta-analyses have been conducted, at times reaching different conclusions. A 2001 meta-analysis reviewing the relationship between video game violence and aggression in teenagers (n = 3,033) found a significant and positive correlation, indicating that high video game violence does lead to greater aggression among teenagers.[4]

Another meta-analysis conducted the same year by John Sherry was more skeptical of effects, specifically questioning whether the interactivity of video games made them have more effect than other media.[5] Sherry later published another meta-analysis in 2007, again concluding that the influence of video game violence on aggression was minimal. Sherry also criticized the observed dose-response curve, reporting that smaller effects were found in experimental studies with longer exposure times, where one might expect greater exposure to cause greater effects.[137]

In 2010, Anderson's group published a meta-analysis of one hundred and thirty international studies with over 130,000 participants. He reported that exposure to violent video games caused both short-term and long-term aggression in players and decreased empathy and pro-social behavior.[138] However, other scholars criticized this meta-analysis for excluding non-significant studies and for other methodological flaws.[139][140][141] Anderson's group have defended their analysis, rejecting these critiques.[142] Rowell Huesmann, a psychology and social studies academic at the University of Michigan wrote an editorial supporting the Anderson meta-analysis.[143] A later re-analysis of the Anderson meta-analysis suggested that there was greater publication bias among experiments than Anderson and colleagues had accounted for. This indicated that the effects observed in laboratory experiments may have been smaller than estimated and perhaps not statistically significant.[144] A reply by Anderson and colleagues acknowledged that there was publication bias among experiments, but disagreed that the degree of bias was large enough to bring the effect into question.[145]

A 2015 meta-analysis of video game effects suggested that video games, including violent games, had minimal impact on children's behavior including violence, prosocial behavior and mental health.[146] The journal included a debate section on this meta-analysis including scholars who were both supportive[147] and critical[148][149] of this meta-analysis. The original author also responded to these comments, arguing that few coherent methodological critiques had been raised.[150] In 2016, Kanamori and Doi replicated the original Angry Birds meta-analysis and concluded that critiques of the original meta were largely unwarranted.[151]

In 2018, a meta-analysis of the relationship between violent video game play and physical aggression over time found that "violent video game play is positively associated with aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, and aggressive affect, as well as negatively associated with empathy for victims of violence and with prosocial behavior".[152]

A 2020 meta-analysis of long-term outcome studies concluded that evidence did not support links between earlier playing of violent games and later aggression. The authors found an overall correlation of r = 0.059, and stated that better quality studies were less likely to find evidence for effects than poorer quality studies.[153][154]

fMRI studies

The extent to which video games have a long-term effect on the brain is disputed. Some scientists have attempted to use functional magnetic resonance imaging to study this hypothesis. Some studies suggested that participants who engaged with VVGs displayed increases in the functioning of their amygdala and decreases in the functioning of their frontal lobe.[155] Some scholars argue that the effect on the frontal lobe may be similar to the deactivation seen in disruptive behavior disorders.[156][157] However, potential funding conflicts of interest have been noted for some of these studies. During the Brown Vs. EMA legal case, it was noted that the studies conducted by Kronenberger were openly funded by "The Center for Successful Parenting", which may mean a conflict of interest.[158]

Further, other studies have failed to find a link between violent games and diminished brain function. For example, an fMRI study by Regenbogen and colleagues found no link between violent video games and a diminished ability to differentiate between real and virtual violence.[159] Another study from 2016 using fMRI found no evidence that VVGs led to a desensitization effect in players.[160] In a recent BBC interview, Dr. Simone Kuhn explained that the brain effects seen in prior fMRI studies likely indicated that players were simply able to distinguish between reality and fiction and modulate their emotional reaction accordingly, not becoming desensitized.[161]

Studies on the effect on crime

In 2008, records held by the US Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention and Office of Justice Programs indicated that arrests for violent crime in the US had decreased since the early 1990s in both children and adults.[162][163][164] This decrease occurred contemporaneously with increasing sales of violent video games and increases in graphically violent content in those games.[165][166]

Studies of violent video game playing and crime have generally not supported the existence of causal links. Evidence from studies of juveniles[167][168][169] as well as criminal offenders[170] has generally not uncovered evidence for links. Some studies have suggested that violent video game playing may be associated with reductions in some types of aggression, such as bullying.[171]

Studies of mass shootings have, likewise, provided no evidence for links with violent video games. A 2002 report from the US Secret Service found that school shooters appeared to consume relatively low levels of violent media.[172] Some criminologists have referred to claims linking violent video games to mass shootings as a "myth".[173]

Some studies have examined the consumption of violent video games in society and violent crime rates. Generally, it is acknowledged that societal violent video game consumption has been associated with over an 80% reduction in youth violence in the US during the corresponding period.[174] However, scholars note that, while this data is problematic for arguments that violent video games increase crime, such data is correlational and cannot be used to conclude video games have caused this decline in crime.[175]

Other studies have examined data on violent video games and crime trends more closely and have come to the conclusion that the release of very popular violent video games are causally associated with corresponding declines in violent crime in the short term. A 2011 study by the Center for European Economic Research[176] found that violent video games may be reducing crime. This is possibly because the time spent playing games reduces time spent engaged in more antisocial activities. Other recent studies by Patrick Markey[177] and Scott Cunningham[178] have come to similar conclusions.

Public debate in US

Jack Thompson, an activist, filed lawsuits against the makers of violent games, alleging that simulated violence causes real-world violence.

In the early 1980s, Ronnie Lamm, the president of the Long Island PTA sought legislation to govern the proximity of video game arcades to schools.[179] In the 1990s, Joe Lieberman, a US Senator, chaired a hearing about violent video games such as Mortal Kombat.[180] David Grossman, a former West Point psychology lecturer and lieutenant colonel, wrote books about violence in the media including: On Killing (1996) and Stop Teaching Our Kids to Kill (1999).[181] He described first-person shooter games as murder simulators, and argued that video game publishers unethically train children in the use of weapons and harden them emotionally towards commitments of murder by simulating the killing of hundreds or thousands of opponents in a single typical video game.[182]

In 2003, Craig A. Anderson, a researcher who testified on the topic before the U.S. Senate, said,

"[S]ome studies have yielded nonsignificant video game effects, just as some smoking studies failed to find a significant link to lung cancer. But when one combines all relevant empirical studies using meta-analytic techniques, it shows that violent video games are significantly associated with: increased aggressive behavior, thoughts, and affect; increased physiological arousal; and decreased pro-social (helping) behavior."[183][184][185][186]

In 2005, Anderson was criticized in court for failing to give balanced expert evidence.[187]

In 2008, in Grand Theft Childhood: The Surprising Truth About Violent Video Games and What Parents Can Do, Kutner and Olsen refuted claims that violent video games cause an increase in violent behavior in children. They report there is a scientifically non-significant trend showing that adolescents who do not play video games at all are most at risk for violent behavior and video game play is part of an adolescent boy's normal social setting. However, the authors did not completely deny the negative influences of violent (M-rated) video games on pre-teens and teenagers: Kutner and Olson suggested the views of alarmists and those of representatives of the video game industry are often supported by flawed or misconstrued studies and that the factors leading to violence in children and adolescents were more subtle than whether or not they played violent video games.[188][189]

Henry Jenkins, an academic in media studies, said,

"According to federal crime statistics, the rate of juvenile violent crime in the United States is at a 30-year low. Researchers find that people serving time for violent crimes typically consume less media before committing their crimes than the average person in the general population. It's true that young offenders who have committed school shootings in America have also been game players. But young people in general are more likely to be gamers—90 percent of boys and 40 percent of girls play. The overwhelming majority of kids who play do not commit antisocial acts. According to a 2001 U.S. Surgeon General's report, the strongest risk factors for school shootings centered on mental stability and the quality of home life, not media exposure. The moral panic over violent video games is doubly harmful. It has led adult authorities to be more suspicious and hostile to many kids who already feel cut off from the system. It also misdirects energy away from eliminating the actual causes of youth violence and allows problems to continue to fester."[190]

In 2013, Corey Mead, a professor of English at Baruch College, wrote about how the U.S. military financed the original development of video games, and has long used them for both training, recruitment purposes, and treatment of post traumatic stress disorder. He also argues that the two industries are currently intertwined into each other in a "military-entertainment complex".[191] Writing in 2013, scholars James Ivory and Malte Elson noted that, although research on video game effects remained inconclusive, the culture of the academic field itself had become very contentious and that politicians had put pressure on scientists to produce specific research findings. The authors concluded it is improper for scholars or legislators to, at present, portray video games as a public health crisis.[113] Research by Oxford psychologist Andrew Przybylski has shown that Americans are split in opinion on how video game violence links to gun violence. Przybylski found that older people, women rather than men, people who knew less about games and who were very conservative in ideology were most likely to think video games could cause gun violence.[192]

Several groups address video game violence as a topic that they focus on. Groups such as Parents Against Violence, Parents Against Media Violence and One Million Moms take stances aimed at limiting the violence in video games and other media.[193][194][195]

Video games, particularly violent ones, are often mentioned as a cause for major gun crimes in the wake of school shooting by young adults. For example, Adam Lanza, the 20-year-old shooter at the Sandy Hook Elementary School shooting, was found to have numerous video games in his possession, leading for some people to blame video games for the shooting;[196] however, the State Attorney did not link video game to the event in their final report of the incident, though identified that video game addiction may have been connected.[197][198] In February 2018, following the Stoneman Douglas High School shooting in Florida, President Donald Trump, among others, said "the level of violence on video games is really shaping young people's thoughts".[199] Rhode Island state representative Robert Nardolillo also proposed legislation to tax violent video games (those rated "Mature" or higher by the ESRB) to use funds for supporting mental health programs in the state.[200]

Following the Stoneman Douglas shooting event, President Trump arranged to meet with several video game industry professionals on March 8, 2018; in attendance beyond Trump and other Congressmen included Mike Gallagher, the president and CEO of the ESA; Pat Vance, the president of the ESRB; Strauss Zelnick, CEO of Take Two Interactive, Robert Altman, CEO of ZeniMax Media; Brent Bozell, founder of the Media Research Center; and Melissa Hanson, program manager for the Parents Television Council. The meeting was not designed to come to a solution but only for the invited parties to present their stance on video games and their relationship to violent activity as to try to determine appropriate steps in the future.[201] At the start of the meeting, the President showed the attendees a short 88-second video of numerous violent video game segments put together by his staff, including the infamous "No Russian" level from Call of Duty: Modern Warfare 2, which featured the player watching and potentially participating in a massacre of civilians in an airport.[202]

The White House later released the video to YouTube, where it quickly became popular due to the controversy over the relationship between video games and real-life violence; despite being unlisted shortly after being uploaded, it has reached about 1.6 million views as of 2023.[203][204] The video is still accessible via URL, and media outlets like IGN included links to the original in their responses to the matter.[205] Games for Change made a short response video with 550.000 views titled "#GameOn".[206]

Nation factors

Australia

Video games are rated in Australia by the Australian Classification Board (ACB), run out of the federal Attorney-General's Department. ACB also oversees ratings on films and applies the same ratings system as to video games. Broadly, the ratings system is based on a number of factors including violence. The ACB can refuse to classify a film or game if they felt the content was beyond allowable guidelines for the strictest ratings. Titles refused classification by ACB are thus illegal to sell within Australia and assess fines for those that attempted to import such games, while allowing titles with more mature ratings to be sold under regulated practices. Prior to 2011, video games could only qualify up to a "MA15+" rating, and not the next highest tier of "R18+" which were allowed for film. Several high-profile games thus were banned in Australia. The ACB agreed to allow video games to have R18+ ratings in 2011, and some of these games that were previously banned were subsequently allowed under R18+.

References

  1. ^ "APA RESOLUTION on Violent Video Games" (PDF). www.apa.org. Retrieved November 5, 2022.
  2. ^ "APA TASK FORCE REPORT on Violent Video Games" (PDF). www.apa.org. Retrieved November 5, 2022.
  3. ^ Markey, Patrick; Ferguson, Christopher (2017). "2". Moral Combat: Why the War on Violent Video Games Is Wrong. BenBella Books, Inc. ISBN 978-1942952992.
  4. ^ a b c d Anderson, Craig A.; Bushman, Brad J. (2001). "Effects of violent video games on aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive affect, physiological arousal, and prosocial behavior: A meta-analytic review of the scientific literature". Psychological Science. 12 (5): 353–359. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00366. PMID 11554666. S2CID 14987425.
  5. ^ a b Sherry, John (2001). "The effects of violent video games on aggression: a meta-analysis". Human Communication Research. 27 (3): 409–431. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2001.tb00787.x. S2CID 6322160.
  6. ^ Ferguson, Christopher J.; Kilburn, J. (2009). "The public health risks of media violence: a meta-analytic review". The Journal of Pediatrics. 154 (5): 759–763. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.11.033. PMID 19230901.
  7. ^ a b Anderson CA, Gentile DA, Buckley KE (2007). Violent Video Game Effects on Children and Adolescents. Theory, Research, and Public Policy. New York: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195309836.001.0001. ISBN 978-0-19-530983-6.
  8. ^ Radoff, Jon (December 8, 2009). "Six wonderful things about video games". Radoff.com. Archived from the original on December 13, 2009. Retrieved March 8, 2014.
  9. ^ Ferguson, C. J.; Garza, A. (2011). "Call of (civic) duty: Action games and civic behavior in a large sample of youth". Computers in Human Behavior. 27 (2): 770–775. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2010.10.026.
  10. ^ Cumberbatch, Guy (2004). "Video violence: villain or victim?" (paper). London: Video Standards Council. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  11. ^ Sternheimer, Karen (2003). It's not the media: the truth about pop culture's influence on children. Boulder, Colorado: Westview Press. ISBN 978-0813341385.
  12. ^ Benedetti, Winda (February 18, 2008). "Why search our souls when video games make such an easy scapegoat?". NBC News. Retrieved August 17, 2008.
  13. ^ Newzoo PC & Console Gaming Report 2023, pp. 37. Results from: Newzoo Global Gamer Study 2022 (Global weighted average across 36 markets; n=60,020).
  14. ^ Martha Irvine (October 17, 2008). "Survey: 97 Percent Of Children Play Video Games". Huffingtonpost.com. Archived from the original on January 7, 2010. Retrieved February 18, 2015.
  15. ^ Abad-Santos, Alex (March 13, 2015). "The insane history of how American paranoia ruined and censored comic books". Vox. Retrieved August 25, 2019.
  16. ^ Burrus, Trevor (March 9, 2018). "These Kids Today: The Long History of Complaining About Violent Entertainment". Cato Institute. Retrieved August 25, 2019.
  17. ^ Good, Owen (January 11, 2013). "Comic Books' Disastrous Self-Censorship Offers a Lesson for Games Publishers, Too". Kotaku. Retrieved August 25, 2019.
  18. ^ June, Laura (January 1, 2013). "For Amusement Only: the life and death of the American arcade". The Verge. Retrieved August 11, 2020.
  19. ^ Smith, Ryan (May 5, 2018). "Chicago once waged a 40-year war on pinball". Chicago Reader. Retrieved August 11, 2020.
  20. ^ a b c d e f Kocurek, Carly (September 2012). "The Agony and the Exidy: A History of Video Game Violence and the Legacy of Death Race". Game Studies. 12 (1). Retrieved August 11, 2020.
  21. ^ Walker, Wendy (July 2, 1976). "It Offers That Run-Down Feeling". Associated Press.
  22. ^ Blumenthal, Ralph (December 28, 1976). "Death Race". The New York Times. Retrieved August 22, 2019.
  23. ^ "AROUND THE NATION; Surgeon General Sees Danger in Video Games". The New York Times. Associated Press. November 10, 1982. Retrieved August 22, 2019.
  24. ^ Mandel, Howard (March 1983). "The Great Debate". VideoGames & Computer Entertainment. Vol. 72. pp. 21–24.
  25. ^ Fahs, Travis (May 5, 2011). "The History of Mortal Kombat. Follow its bloody legacy from 1992 to the present day". IGN. Retrieved August 22, 2019.
  26. ^ a b c Crossley, Rob (June 2, 2014). "Mortal Kombat: Violent game that changed video games industry". BBC. Archived from the original on August 1, 2018. Retrieved October 30, 2018.
  27. ^ Scolars, Mike (August 22, 2017). "Nintendo vs. Sega: The battle over being cool". Polygon. Retrieved October 31, 2018.
  28. ^ Redburn, Tom (December 17, 1993). "Toys 'R' Us Stops Selling a Violent Video Game". The New York Times. Retrieved October 31, 2018.
  29. ^ Cohen, Karen (March 3, 1994). "Video game makers say they are making progress on ratings". UPI. Retrieved November 2, 2018.
  30. ^ a b Kohler, Chris (July 29, 2009). "July 29, 1994: Videogame Makers Propose Ratings Board to Congress". Wired. Condé Nast Publications. Archived from the original on February 18, 2014. Retrieved June 1, 2011.
  31. ^ Buckley, Sean (June 6, 2013). "Then there were three: Sony, Microsoft, Nintendo and the evolution of the Electronic Entertainment Expo". Engadget. Retrieved May 9, 2017.
  32. ^ a b c d Bella, Timothy (August 5, 2019). "Politicians suggest video games are to blame for the El Paso shooting. It's an old claim that's not backed by research". The Washington Post. Retrieved August 22, 2019.
  33. ^ Vossekuil, Bryan; Fein, Robert; Reddy, Marisa; Borum, Randy; Modzeleski, William (July 2004). "The Final Report and Findings of the Safe School Initiative: Implications for the Prevention of School Attacks in the United States" (PDF). United States Department of Education. Retrieved August 22, 2019.
  34. ^ Kent S. "The Mainstream and All Its Perils" The ultimate history of video games:... Three Rivers Press, New York 2001 p544–545. ISBN 0761536434
  35. ^ Barr S. and Grapes B. "Violent children." Greenhaven Press, San Diego 2000 p26 ISBN 0737701595 (originally printed as "Computer Violence: Are Your Kids At Risk?" Reader's Digest January 1999.)
  36. ^ Radwick, Dora; Dolan, Sean P. (February 7, 2002). "NPD Reports Annual 2001 U.S. Interactive Entertainment Sales Shatter Industry Record" (Press release). Port Washington, New York: NPD Group. Archived from the original on August 14, 2004. Retrieved April 9, 2016.
  37. ^ Good, Owen (October 23, 2011). "Grand Theft Auto III: The Birth Of A Media Scapegoat". Kotaku. Gawker Media. Archived from the original on April 22, 2016. Retrieved December 21, 2015.
  38. ^ Richtel, Matt (February 10, 2002). "BACKSLASH; Mayhem, and Far From the Nicest Kind". The New York Times. Archived from the original on April 21, 2016. Retrieved December 21, 2015.
  39. ^ Cohen, Adam. “California's Misguided War on Violent Video Games.” Time, Time Inc., 27 October 2010, content.time.com/time/nation/article/0,8599,2027692,00.html.
  40. ^ Benson, Julian (September 15, 2015). "The Rise and Fall of Video Gaming's Most Vocal Enemy". Kotaku. Retrieved August 22, 2019.
  41. ^ "Verwirrung um Therapie von Tim K. – Eltern des Amokschützen dementieren Behandlung – Schütze spielte vor der Tat Killerspiel" (in German). news-times.de. March 11, 2009. Retrieved March 19, 2009.
  42. ^ "Polizei hat Hinweise auf Tatmotiv". Der Spiegel (in German). March 12, 2009. Retrieved March 12, 2009.
  43. ^ "CSU will Killerspiel-Verbot, SPD Kontrolle über Schützenvereine". Der Spiegel (in German). March 13, 2009. Retrieved March 13, 2009.
  44. ^ "Opferfamilien fordern Killerspiel-Verbot" (in German). sueddeutsche.de. March 21, 2009. Retrieved March 21, 2009.
  45. ^ "Winnenden: Eine Tat, aus der man nicht klug werden kann". http://www.welt.de (in German). March 21, 2009. Retrieved March 24, 2009.
  46. ^ Thorsen, Tor (October 29, 2009). "Modern Warfare 2 massacre 'not representative of overall experience' - Activision". GameSpot. Archived from the original on August 20, 2018. Retrieved October 8, 2018.
  47. ^ Stuart, Keith (October 29, 2009). "Should Modern Warfare 2 allow us to play at terrorism?". The Guardian. Archived from the original on August 26, 2016. Retrieved August 1, 2016.
  48. ^ Orry, James (November 10, 2009). "BBC reporter 'saddened' but not 'shocked' by MW2". VideoGamer.com. Candy Banana. Archived from the original on August 17, 2016. Retrieved August 1, 2016.
  49. ^ Parker, Laura (June 26, 2012). "Is It Time for Games to Get Serious?". GameSpot. CBS Interactive. Archived from the original on February 22, 2016. Retrieved August 1, 2016.
  50. ^ Hester, Blake (October 28, 2019). ""Remember, No Russian:" Critics and Developers Remember Call of Duty's Most Infamous Mission". USGamer. Retrieved October 28, 2019.
  51. ^ "Judge rules Michigan video game law is unconstitutional". USA Today. April 4, 2006. Retrieved November 1, 2010.
  52. ^ Broache, Anne (November 26, 2006). "Court rejects Illinois video game law". CNet. Archived from the original on January 20, 2013. Retrieved November 1, 2010.
  53. ^ Kravits, David (June 27, 2011). "States May Not Ban Sale, Rental of Violent Videogames to Minors". Wired. Archived from the original on June 29, 2011. Retrieved June 27, 2011.
  54. ^ Kendall, Brent (November 2, 2010). "Court Voices Doubts on Violent Videogame Law". Wall Street Journal. Archived from the original on July 24, 2015. Retrieved November 2, 2010.
  55. ^ Jaslow, Ryan (February 18, 2013). "Violent video games and mass violence: A complex link". CBS News. Retrieved August 22, 2019.
  56. ^ Ferguson, Christopher J. (February 20, 2013). "Video games didn't cause Newtown rampage". CNN. Retrieved November 11, 2013.
  57. ^ Lohr, David (December 17, 2012). "Sandy Hook Blame Game: Eeny, Meeny, Miny, Moe". Huffington Post. Retrieved November 11, 2013.
  58. ^ Ferguson, Christopher J. (December 20, 2012). "Sandy Hook Shooting: Video Games Blamed, Again". Time. Retrieved November 11, 2013.
  59. ^ Robertson, Adi (January 12, 2013). "Joe Biden meets with gaming industry over gun violence, but promises he's not singling it out". The Verge. Retrieved April 30, 2020.
  60. ^ Beekman, Daniel (December 21, 2012). "NRA blames video games like 'Kindergarten Killer' for Sandy Hook slaughter". Daily News. New York. Retrieved November 11, 2013.
  61. ^ Winda Benedetti (December 21, 2012). "NRA: Video game industry 'sows violence against its own people'". NBC News. NBCUniversal News Group. Archived from the original on June 22, 2022. Retrieved May 4, 2023.
  62. ^ Kreienbrink, Matthias (July 26, 2016). "Killerspiele: Schon wieder die K-Frage". Die Zeit (in German). ISSN 0044-2070. Retrieved January 21, 2020.
  63. ^ Brühl, Jannis (July 23, 2016). "Amoklauf: De Maizière und die Killerspiel-Debatte". Süddeutsche.de (in German). Retrieved January 21, 2020.
  64. ^ Wartman, Scott. "School shootings: Kentucky Gov. Matt Bevin blames violent video games and shows, not guns". The Cincinnati Enquirer. Retrieved April 17, 2018.
  65. ^ Fogel, Stefanie. "Kentucky Governor Blames Video Games for Florida School Shooting". Rolling Stone. Archived from the original on April 17, 2018. Retrieved April 17, 2018.
  66. ^ Snider, Mike (March 8, 2018). "These are the video games the White House played in its meeting on game violence". USA Today. Retrieved August 22, 2019.
  67. ^ Bittar, Bernardo (March 13, 2019). "Mourão sobre Suzano: 'Jovens estão muito viciados em videogames violentos'". Correio Braziliense (in Brazilian Portuguese). Retrieved January 21, 2020.
  68. ^ "Do violent videogames influence young people? What researches show | Texto em inglês com áudio". Wise Up News: textos em inglês com áudio da Gazeta do Povo. March 19, 2019. Retrieved January 21, 2020.
  69. ^ a b c Draper, Kevin (August 5, 2019). "Video Games Aren't Why Shootings Happen. Politicians Still Blame Them". The New York Times. Retrieved August 22, 2019.
  70. ^ Haselton, Todd; Graham, Megan (October 9, 2019). "About 2,200 people watched the German synagogue shooting on Amazon's Twitch". CNBC. Retrieved January 21, 2020.
  71. ^ Hurtz, Simon; dpa (October 13, 2019). "Anschlag in Halle: Horst Seehofer reanimiert die Killerspieldebatte". Die Zeit (in German). ISSN 0044-2070. Retrieved January 21, 2020.
  72. ^ "Die Neunziger wollen ihre Killerspiel-Debatte zurück". Der Tagesspiegel Online (in German). October 14, 2019. Retrieved January 21, 2020.
  73. ^ Gobernador de Coahuila culpa a los videojuegos por el tiroteo de Torreón am.com.mx, Jan 10, 2020 Natural Selection, el videojuego que jugaba autor de tiroteo en Torreón Milenio Digital, Jan 10, 2020
  74. ^ Aclara experto de la UNAM que videojuegos no provocan tiroteos PalcoNoticias, Jan 11, 2020
  75. ^ Los videojuegos no tienen la culpa; los padres sí: Especialista By Amelia Obregón, El Imparcial (Mexico), Jan 10, 2020
  76. ^ Crisp, James (June 30, 2023). "Teenagers in French riots are copying video games, says Emmanuel Macron". The Telegraph. ISSN 0307-1235. Retrieved July 2, 2023.
  77. ^ a b "Technical Report on the Review of The Violent Video Game Literature" (PDF). American Psychological Association. 2015. Retrieved April 12, 2021.
  78. ^ Chris Ferguson; Dana Klisinan; Jerri Lynn Hogg; June Wilson; Patrick Markey; Andy Przybylski; Malte Elson; Jimmy Ivory; Deborah Linebarger; Mary Gregerson; Frank Farley; Shahbaz Siddiqui (June 12, 2017). "News Media, Public Education and Public Policy Committee". The Amplifier Magazine. American Psychological Association. Retrieved August 22, 2019.
  79. ^ "APA Reaffirms Position on Violent Video Games and Violent Behavior". American Psychological Association. March 3, 2020. Retrieved March 4, 2020.
  80. ^ Bogost, Ian (August 5, 2019). "Video-Game Violence Is Now a Partisan Issue". The Atlantic. Retrieved August 22, 2019.
  81. ^ Ferguson, Christopher; Wang, JCK (August 2019). "Aggressive Video Games are Not a Risk Factor for Future Aggression in Youth: A Longitudinal Study". Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 48 (8): 1439–1451. doi:10.1007/s10964-019-01069-0. PMID 31273603. S2CID 195807704.
  82. ^ Smith, S; Ferguson, C; Beaver, K (May 2018). "A longitudinal analysis of shooter games and their relationship with conduct disorder and cself-reported delinquency". International Journal of Law and Psychiatry. 58: 48–53. doi:10.1016/j.ijlp.2018.02.008. PMID 29853012. S2CID 46919245.
  83. ^ Kühn, Simone; Kugler, Dimitrij Tycho; Schmalen, Katharina; Weichenberger, Markus; Witt, Charlotte; Gallinat, Jürgen (August 2019). "Does playing violent video games cause aggression? A longitudinal intervention study". Molecular Psychiatry. 24 (8): 1220–1234. doi:10.1038/s41380-018-0031-7. ISSN 1476-5578. PMC 6756088. PMID 29535447.
  84. ^ Dolan, Eric W. (December 3, 2023). "Brain imaging study challenges long-held beliefs about negative effects of violent video games". PsyPost. Retrieved January 22, 2024.
  85. ^ DeWall, Anderson & Bushman (2011). "The General Aggression Model: Theoretical Extensions to Violence" (PDF). Psychology of Violence. 1 (3): 245–258. doi:10.1037/a0023842. Archived from the original (PDF) on August 12, 2017. Retrieved January 3, 2017.
  86. ^ Kooijmans T. "Effects of video games on aggressive thoughts and behaviors during development."Rochester Institute of Technology December 2004.
  87. ^ Ferguson & Dyck (2012). "Paradigm change in aggression research: The time has come to retire the General Aggression Model" (PDF). Aggression and Violent Behavior. 17 (3): 220–228. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2012.02.007.
  88. ^ Ballard, Mary; Visser, Kara; Jocoy, Kathleen (November 19, 2012). "Social Context and Video Game Play: Impact on Cardiovascular and Affective Responses". Mass Communication and Society. 15 (6): 875–898. doi:10.1080/15205436.2011.632106. ISSN 1532-7825. S2CID 144482524.
  89. ^ Sauer, James D.; Drummond, Aaron; Nova, Natalie (September 1, 2015). "Violent video games: The effects of narrative context and reward structure on in-game and postgame aggression". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 21 (3): 205–214. doi:10.1037/xap0000050. ISSN 1939-2192. PMID 26121373. S2CID 40548080.
  90. ^ a b McCarthy, Randy J.; Coley, Sarah L.; Wagner, Michael F.; Zengel, Bettina; Basham, Ariel (November 1, 2016). "Does playing video games with violent content temporarily increase aggressive inclinations? A pre-registered experimental study". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. Special Issue: Confirmatory. 67: 13–19. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2015.10.009.
  91. ^ Ferguson C. et al. "Violent video games and aggression: Causal relationship or byproduct of family violence and intrinsic violence motivation?" Criminal Justice and Behavior 2008 35 p311–332.
  92. ^ Ferguson, C.; Beaver, K. (2009). "Natural born killers: the genetic origins of extreme violence, aggression and violent behavior". Aggression and Violent Behavior. 14 (5): 286–294. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2009.03.005.
  93. ^ Schulzke M. "Moral decision making in fallout." Game Studies 2009 9(2). Accessed 29 November 2013.
  94. ^ Gentile D. et al. "The effects of prosocial video games on prosocial behaviors: international evidence from correlational, longitudinal and experimental studies." National Institute of Health. Accessed 24 April 2013.
  95. ^ Studies supporting the hypothesis that playing violent video games increases aggression in young people:
  96. ^ Rothmund, Tobias; Bender, Jens; Nauroth, Peter; Gollwitzer, Mario (2015). "Public concerns about violent video games are moral concerns—How moral threat can make pacifists susceptible to scientific and political claims against violent video games". European Journal of Social Psychology. 45 (6): 769–783. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2125. ISSN 1099-0992.
  97. ^ Studies that didn't find a link between the two:
  98. ^ a b Tear, Morgan J.; Nielsen, Mark (2013). "Failure to Demonstrate That Playing Violent Video Games Diminishes Prosocial Behavior". PLOS ONE. 8 (7): e68382. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...868382T. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0068382. PMC 3700923. PMID 23844191.
  99. ^ Bushman, Brad; Gollwitzer, Mario; Cruz, Carlos (2014). "There is broad consensus: Media researchers agree that violent media increase aggression in children, and pediatricians and parents concur". Psychology of Popular Media Culture.
  100. ^ Etchells, Pete; Chambers, Chris (October 10, 2014). "Violent video games research: consensus or confusion?". The Guardian. ISSN 0261-3077. Retrieved January 3, 2017.
  101. ^ Kirsh, Steven (1998). "Seeing the world through Mortal Kombat-colored glasses: Violent video games and the development of a short-term Hostile Attribution Bias". Childhood. 5 (2): 177–184. doi:10.1177/0907568298005002005. S2CID 143735522.
  102. ^ Anderson, Craig A.; Karen E., Dill (April 2000). "Video games and aggressive thoughts, feelings, and behavior in the laboratory and in life". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (4): 772–790. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.4.772. PMID 10794380. S2CID 9606759.
  103. ^ Ferguson; et al. (2008). "VIOLENT VIDEO GAMES AND AGGRESSION Causal Relationship or Byproduct of Family Violence and Intrinsic Violence Motivation?" (PDF). Criminal Justice and Behavior. 35 (3): 311–332. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.494.950. doi:10.1177/0093854807311719. S2CID 7145392.
  104. ^ Wright B. "Sounding the alarm on video game ratings." CNN website 18 February 2004. Accessed 10 December 2004.
  105. ^ Vosekuill B. "Safe school initiative final report." U.S. Secret Service and U.S. Department of Education May 2002 p26.
  106. ^ Ferguson, Christopher J. (January 1, 2008). "The school shooting/violent video game link: causal relationship or moral panic?". Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling. 5 (1–2): 25–37. doi:10.1002/jip.76. ISSN 1544-4767. S2CID 62812428.
  107. ^ Funk, J.B.; Buchman, D.D.; Jenks, J.; Bechtoldt, H. (2003). "Playing violent video games, desensitization, and moral evaluation in children". Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 24 (4): 413–416. doi:10.1016/S0193-3973(03)00073-X.
  108. ^ Funk J. B.; et al. (2004). "Violence exposure in real-life, video games, television, movies, and the internet: Is there desensitization?" (PDF). Journal of Adolescence. 27 (27): 23–39. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.10.005. PMID 15013258. Archived from the original (PDF) on March 27, 2014. Retrieved April 23, 2015.
  109. ^ Colwell, John; Kato, Makiko (August 1, 2003). "Investigation of the relationship between social isolation, self-esteem, aggression and computer game play in Japanese adolescents". Asian Journal of Social Psychology. 6 (2): 149–158. doi:10.1111/1467-839X.t01-1-00017. ISSN 1467-839X. S2CID 144813700.
  110. ^ "On Media Theory...: UPDATE: Four Respond to 228, Responding to the APA: Dissent Within the Academy Regarding Media Violence (#ECA14 Panel)". Onmediatheory.blogspot.com. Retrieved February 17, 2015.
  111. ^ Ferguson, Christopher J. (2013). "Violent video games and the Supreme Court: Lessons for the scientific community in the wake of Brown v. Entertainment Merchants Association". American Psychologist. 68 (2): 57–74. doi:10.1037/a0030597. PMID 23421606.
  112. ^ "Scholar's Open Letter to the APA Task Force On Violent Media Opposing APA Policy Statements on Violent Media". Scribd.com. Retrieved February 17, 2015.
  113. ^ a b "A Hornet's Nest Over Violent Video Games – The Conversation - Blogs - The Chronicle of Higher Education". Chronicle.com. October 16, 2013. Retrieved February 17, 2015.
  114. ^ B.D. Bartholow, et al. Chronic violent video game exposure and desensitization to violence: Behavioral and event-related brain potential data
  115. ^ Nicholas L. Carnagey, et al. The effect of video game violence on physiological desensitization to real-life violence Archived 2013-05-16 at the Wayback Machine
  116. ^ "Kids unaffected by violent games." Wired, 2 April 2007.
  117. ^ Shibuya, A.; Sakamoto, A.; Ihori, N.; Yukawa, S. (2008). "The effects of the presence and contexts of video game violence in children: A longitudinal study in Japan". Simulation & Gaming. 39 (4): 528–539. doi:10.1177/1046878107306670. S2CID 145062731.
  118. ^ "Teens, Video Games and Civics". Pew Research Center's Internet & American Life Project. September 16, 2008. Retrieved February 17, 2015.
  119. ^ Christopher J. Ferguson; Adolfo Garza (March 2011). "Call of (civic) duty: Action games and civic behavior in a large sample of youth" (PDF). Computers in Human Behavior. 27 (2): 770–775. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2010.10.026. Archived from the original (PDF) on May 19, 2014.
  120. ^ Douglas A. Gentile; et al. (2009). "The Effects of Prosocial Video Games on Prosocial Behaviors: International Evidence From Correlational, Longitudinal, and Experimental Studies". Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 35 (6): 752–63. doi:10.1177/0146167209333045. PMC 2678173. PMID 19321812.
  121. ^ "Video Game Violence Use Among "Vulnerability to violent video games: A review and integration of personality research."
  122. ^ a b "Literature review on the impact of playing violent video games on aggression" Australian government 2010.
  123. ^ Adachi, Paul J. C.; Willoughby, Teena (January 1, 2011). "The effect of violent video games on aggression: Is it more than just the violence?". Aggression and Violent Behavior. 16 (1): 55–62. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2010.12.002. S2CID 143649264.
  124. ^ Adachi & Willoughby (2013). "The Effect of Video Game Competition and Violence on Aggressive Behavior: Which Characteristic Has the Greatest Influence?" (PDF). Psychology of Violence.
  125. ^ Sarah H. Konrath. Edward H. O'Brien. Courtney Hsing. "Changes in Dispositional Empathy in American College Students Over Time: A Meta-Analysis" Personality and Social Psychology Review
  126. ^ Von Salisch, M.; et al. (2011). "Preference for violent electronic games and aggressive behavior among children: the beginning of the downward spiral?". Media Psychology. 14 (3): 233–258. doi:10.1080/15213269.2011.596468. S2CID 145764509.
  127. ^ Bennerstedt, Ulrika; Ivarsson, Jonas; Linderoth, Jonas (2011). "How gamers manage aggression: Situating skills in collaborative computer games". International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning. 7 (1): 43–61. doi:10.1007/s11412-011-9136-6. S2CID 1595007.
  128. ^ Granic G. et al. "The Benefits of Playing Video Games" APA 2013.
  129. ^ Ferguson, Christopher J.; Olson, Cheryl K. (2013). "Video Game Violence Use Among "Vulnerable" Populations: The Impact of Violent Games on Delinquency and Bullying Among Children with Clinically Elevated Depression or Attention Deficit Symptoms". Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 43 (1): 127–136. doi:10.1007/s10964-013-9986-5. PMID 23975351. S2CID 207207723.
  130. ^ "Villanova Professor: Violent video games don't cause real life violence in most cases". www.timesherald.com. September 30, 2014. Retrieved October 20, 2015.
  131. ^ Przybylski, Andrew (2014). "Competence-impeding electronic games and players' aggressive feelings, thoughts, and behaviors". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 106 (3): 441–457. doi:10.1037/a0034820. PMID 24377357. S2CID 5651868.
  132. ^ Hull, Jay G.; Brunelle, Timothy J.; Prescott, Anna T.; Sargent, James D. (August 1, 2014). "A longitudinal study of risk-glorifying video games and behavioral deviance". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 107 (2): 300–325. doi:10.1037/a0036058. ISSN 1939-1315. PMC 4151190. PMID 25090130.
  133. ^ "APA Review Confirms Link Between Playing Violent Video Games and Aggression". American Psychological Association. August 13, 2015. Retrieved December 16, 2015.
  134. ^ "APA Says Video Games Make You Violent, but Critics Cry Bias". Newsweek. August 20, 2015. Retrieved December 19, 2015.
  135. ^ Engelhardt, Christopher (2015). "Effects of violent-video-game exposure on aggressive behavior, aggressive-thought accessibility, and aggressive affect among adults with and without autism spectrum disorder" (PDF). Psychological Science. 26 (8): 1187–1200. doi:10.1177/0956797615583038. PMID 26113064. S2CID 17630510.
  136. ^ Gabbiadini, Alessandro; Riva, Paolo; Andrighetto, Luca; Volpato, Chiara; Bushman, Brad J. (April 13, 2016). "Acting like a Tough Guy: Violent-Sexist Video Games, Identification with Game Characters, Masculine Beliefs, & Empathy for Female Violence Victims". PLOS ONE. 11 (4): e0152121. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1152121G. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0152121. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 4830454. PMID 27074057.
  137. ^ Sherry J. "Mass media effects research: advances through meta-analysis." 2007 p244. "Violent Video Games and Aggression: Why Can't We Find Effects?"
  138. ^ Anderson C. et al. "Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior in Eastern and Western countries." Psychological Bulletin 2010 136 p151–173.
  139. ^ Ferguson C. "Blazing angels or resident evil: can violent video games be a force for good?" Review of General Psychology 14 p68–81.
  140. ^ Ferguson C. and Kilburn J. "Much ado about nothing: the mis-estimation and overinterpretation of violent video game effects in eastern and western nations. A comment on Anderson et al. (2010)." Psychological Bulletin 2010 136(2) p174–178.
  141. ^ "Violence & Video Games: A Weak, Meaningless Correlation". Psych Central.com. Retrieved February 17, 2015.
  142. ^ Anderson C. et al. "Much ado about something: violent video game effects and a school of red herring. A reply to Ferguson and Kilburn (2010)." Psychological Bulletin 2010 136 (2) p182–187
  143. ^ L. Rowell Huesmann: "Nailing the coffin shut on doubts that violent video games stimulate aggression"
  144. ^ "APA PsycNet".
  145. ^ "APA PsycNet".
  146. ^ Psychology, Christopher J. Ferguson Associate Professor of; University, Stetson (October 13, 2015). "Do Angry Birds Make for Angry Children?". The Huffington Post. Retrieved January 3, 2017.
  147. ^ Markey, Patrick (2015). "Finding the Middle Ground in Violent Video Game Research: Lessons From Ferguson (2015)" (PDF). Perspectives on Psychological Science. 10 (5): 667–670. doi:10.1177/1745691615592236. PMID 26386003. S2CID 20511943.
  148. ^ Boxer, P.; Groves, C. L.; Docherty, M. (September 19, 2015). "Video Games Do Indeed Influence Children and Adolescents' Aggression, Prosocial Behavior, and Academic Performance: A Clearer Reading of Ferguson (2015)". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 10 (5): 671–673. doi:10.1177/1745691615592239. ISSN 1745-6924. PMID 26386004. S2CID 206778387.
  149. ^ Rothstein, H. R.; Bushman, B. J. (September 19, 2015). "Methodological and Reporting Errors in Meta-Analytic Reviews Make Other Meta-Analysts Angry: A Commentary on Ferguson (2015)". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 10 (5): 677–679. doi:10.1177/1745691615592235. ISSN 1745-6924. PMID 26386006. S2CID 206778373.
  150. ^ Ferguson, Christopher (2015). "Pay No Attention to That Data Behind the Curtain: On Angry Birds, Happy Children, Scholarly Squabbles, Publication Bias, and Why Betas Rule Metas" (PDF). Perspectives on Psychological Science. 10 (5): 683–691. doi:10.1177/1745691615593353. PMID 26386008. S2CID 9788428.
  151. ^ Kanamori & Doi (2016). "Angry Birds, Angry Children, and Angry Meta-Analysts: A Reanalysis". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 11 (3): 408–14. doi:10.1177/1745691616635599. PMID 27217253. S2CID 37271687.
  152. ^ Prescott, Anna T.; Sargent, James D.; Hull, Jay G. (October 2, 2018). "Metaanalysis of the relationship between violent video game play and physical aggression over time". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 115 (40): 9882–9888. Bibcode:2018PNAS..115.9882P. doi:10.1073/pnas.1611617114. ISSN 1091-6490. PMC 6176643. PMID 30275306.
  153. ^ Drummond, Aaron (2020). "Do longitudinal studies support long-term relationships between aggressive game play and youth aggressive behaviour? A meta-analytic examination". Royal Society Open Science. 7 (7): 200373. Bibcode:2020RSOS....700373D. doi:10.1098/rsos.200373. PMC 7428266. PMID 32874632. S2CID 220666659.
  154. ^ Hern, Alex (July 21, 2020). "Playing video games doesn't lead to violent behaviour, study shows". The Guardian. Retrieved July 31, 2020.
  155. ^ Wendling, Patrice (February 2007). "Violent videos alter brain functioning, study shows". Internal Medicine News. 40 (3): 20. doi:10.1016/s1097-8690(07)70088-4.(subscription required)
  156. ^ Jarrett, Christian (August 2005). "Neural effects of media violence". Psychologist. 18 (8): 462. ProQuest 211742666.
  157. ^ Kalnin, A. J.; Edwards, C. R.; Wang, Y.; Kronenberger, W. G.; Hummer, T. A.; Mosier, K. M.; Mathews, V. P. (2011). "The interacting role of media violence exposure and aggressive-disruptive behavior in adolescent brain activation during an emotional Stroop task". Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 192 (1): 12–19. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.11.005. PMID 21376543. S2CID 45802431.
  158. ^ "Arnold Schwarzenegger. Edmund G. Brown Jr. v. Entertainment Merchants Association, and Entertainment Software Association"
  159. ^ Regenbogen C.; Herrmann M.; Fehr T. (2010). "The neural processing of voluntary completed, real and virtual violent and nonviolent computer game scenarios displaying predefined actions in gamers and nongamers". Society for Neuroscience. 5 (2): 221–40. doi:10.1080/17470910903315989. PMID 19823959. S2CID 205925197.
  160. ^ Szycik, Gregor R.; Mohammadi, Bahram; Hake, Maria; Kneer, Jonas; Samii, Amir; Münte, Thomas F.; Wildt, Bert T. te (April 16, 2016). "Excessive users of violent video games do not show emotional desensitization: an fMRI study". Brain Imaging and Behavior. 11 (3): 736–743. doi:10.1007/s11682-016-9549-y. ISSN 1931-7557. PMID 27086318. S2CID 4399285.
  161. ^ "Are Video Games Really That Bad?, 2014-2015, Horizon - BBC Two". BBC. Retrieved January 3, 2017.
  162. ^ "Juvenile Arrest Rates". Ojjdp.gov. Retrieved February 18, 2015.
  163. ^ "Home". Ojp.usdoj.gov. Archived from the original on December 11, 2009. Retrieved February 17, 2015.
  164. ^ "Home". Ojp.usdoj.gov. Archived from the original on December 11, 2009. Retrieved February 17, 2015.
  165. ^ "Could violent video games reduce rather than increase violence?" Science Daily website 15 May 2008. Accessed 12 July 2011.
  166. ^ Kierkegaard P. "Video games and aggression." International Journal of Liability and Scientific Inquiry p411–417. 2008
  167. ^ Breuer, Johannes; Vogelgesang, Jens; Quandt, Thorsten; Festl, Ruth (October 1, 2015). "Violent video games and physical aggression: Evidence for a selection effect among adolescents". Psychology of Popular Media Culture. 4 (4): 305–328. doi:10.1037/ppm0000035. ISSN 2160-4142.
  168. ^ DeCamp, Whitney (October 1, 2015). "Impersonal agencies of communication: Comparing the effects of video games and other risk factors on violence". Psychology of Popular Media Culture. 4 (4): 296–304. doi:10.1037/ppm0000037. ISSN 2160-4142.
  169. ^ Przybylski, Andrew K.; Mishkin, Allison F. (April 1, 2016). "How the quantity and quality of electronic gaming relates to adolescents' academic engagement and psychosocial adjustment". Psychology of Popular Media Culture. 5 (2): 145–156. doi:10.1037/ppm0000070. ISSN 2160-4142.
  170. ^ Surette, Ray; Maze, Allison (October 1, 2015). "Video game play and copycat crime: An exploratory analysis of an inmate population". Psychology of Popular Media Culture. 4 (4): 360–374. doi:10.1037/ppm0000050. ISSN 2160-4142.
  171. ^ "Video games do not make vulnerable teens more violent". springer.com. Retrieved January 3, 2017.
  172. ^ "Final Report and Findings of the Safe Schools Initiative" (PDF). US Secret Service and US Department of Education. Retrieved January 3, 2017.
  173. ^ Fox, J. A.; DeLateur, M. J. (January 13, 2013). "Mass Shootings in America: Moving Beyond Newtown". Homicide Studies. 18 (1): 125–145. doi:10.1177/1088767913510297. ISSN 1088-7679. S2CID 145184251.
  174. ^ Ferguson, Christopher J. (November 1, 2014). "Does Media Violence Predict Societal Violence? It Depends on What You Look at and When". Journal of Communication. 65 (1): E1–E22. doi:10.1111/jcom.12129. ISSN 1460-2466.
  175. ^ "No link between movie, video game violence and societal violence? More violent video game consumption, less youth violence: Study". www.sciencedaily.com. November 5, 2014. Retrieved January 3, 2017.
  176. ^ Cunningham, A. Scott; Engelstätter, Benjamin; Ward, Michael R. (2011). "Understanding the effects of violent video games on violent crime" (PDF). ZEW Discussion Papers 11-042. Center for European Economic Research. p. 25. Retrieved November 11, 2014.
  177. ^ Markey, Patrick M.; Markey, Charlotte N.; French, Juliana E. (October 2015). "Violent video games and real-world violence: Rhetoric versus data". Psychology of Popular Media Culture. 4 (4): 277–295. doi:10.1037/ppm0000030.
  178. ^ Cunningham, Scott; Engelstätter, Benjamin; Ward, Michael R. (April 1, 2016). "Violent Video Games and Violent Crime". Southern Economic Journal. 82 (4): 1247–1265. doi:10.1002/soej.12139. ISSN 2325-8012.
  179. ^ Gonzalez L. "When two tribes go to war: a history of video game controversy." GameSpot website. Accessed 3 August 2008.
  180. ^ Arnett J. (ed.) and Skalskil P. "Encyclopedia of children, adolescents, and the media: regulation, electronic games." SAGE 2007 p705–707. ISBN 9781412905305, Accessed 11 March 2013.
  181. ^ "Violent video games reward children for killing people." Executive Intelligence Review. LaRouche Publications. Accessed 24 April 2013.
  182. ^ Ferguson C. J. "The school shooting/violent video game link: causal relationship or moral panic?" Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling 2008 5 p25–37.
  183. ^ Anderson C. "Violent video games: myths, facts and unanswered questions." APA 24 October 2003.
  184. ^ Royal, H. (October 1999). "Violence and video games". The Phi Delta Kappan. 81 (2): 173–174.
  185. ^ Anderson C. "The influence of media violence on youth." Psychological science in the public interest website. December 2003.
  186. ^ Lynch P. "The effects of violent video game habits on adolescent aggressive attitudes and behaviors." Society for Research in Child Development April 2001.
  187. ^ "Entertainment Software Association v Illinois" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on March 26, 2009.
  188. ^ Kutner L and Olsen C. Grand theft childhood: the surprising truth about violent video games. 2008. ISBN 0-7432-9951-5
  189. ^ "Interview with Kutner and Olsen" G4 Techdirt website 2008.
  190. ^ "The video game revolution: eight myths about video games debunked." PBS Impact of gaming essays webpage. Date not given. Retrieved 7 March 2014.
  191. ^ Hamza Shaban (October 10, 2013). "Playing War: How the Military Uses Video Games". The Atlantic. Retrieved February 17, 2015.
  192. ^ Andrew Przybylski. "Americans Skeptical of Link Between Mass Shootings and Video Games". Academia.edu. Archived from the original on 11 August 2014. Retrieved 17 February 2015.
  193. ^ "Exposé". Archived from the original on February 20, 2013.
  194. ^ "Purpose of One Million Moms". Archived from the original on August 8, 2014.
  195. ^ "Parents Against Media Violence". Pamv.net. Retrieved February 17, 2015.
  196. ^ Ferguson, Christopher J. (December 20, 2012). "Sandy Hook Shooting: Video Games Blamed, Again". Time. Retrieved November 11, 2013.
  197. ^ "Sandy Hook Final Report. Office of the State's Attorney, Judicial District of Danbury. Stephen J. Sedensky III, State's Attorney" (PDF). ct.gov. November 25, 2013. Retrieved November 28, 2013.
  198. ^ "Adam Lanza's motive a mystery in Sandy Hook killings". CNN. November 27, 2013. Retrieved November 28, 2013.
  199. ^ "Trump says violent video games 'shape' young minds". BBC. BBC News. February 23, 2018. Retrieved February 23, 2018.
  200. ^ Fogel, Stefanie (February 22, 2018). "Rhode Island Lawmaker Wants to 'Do Something Positive' With Proposed Violent Video Game Tax". Glixel. Archived from the original on February 23, 2018. Retrieved February 23, 2018.
  201. ^ Johnson, Ted (March 8, 2018). "Trump Meets With Video Game Industry, Watchdog Groups to Talk Gun Violence". Variety. Retrieved March 12, 2018.
  202. ^ Pereira, Chris (March 10, 2018). "President Trump's Video Games Meeting Included A Violent Game Montage". GameSpot. Retrieved March 12, 2018.
  203. ^ Spangler, Todd (March 9, 2018). "Trump White House's Horribly Violent Video-Game Reel Goes Viral". Variety. Retrieved March 12, 2018.
  204. ^ Violence in Video Games, retrieved April 5, 2018
  205. ^ O'Brien, Lucy (March 9, 2018). "Here's The "Violent Video Game Reel" Trump Shared At The White House". IGN. Retrieved April 5, 2018.
  206. ^ Games for Change (March 12, 2018), #GameOn - 88 Seconds of Video Games, retrieved April 5, 2018

External links